Some
of these resemblances are perfectly staggering—to me they are a source of
constant wonder & thrilling delight. It seems to me as though I obtain a
glimpse of an intelligent motive pervading nature, as well as of the mighty,
never-resting wonder-working laws that regulate all things.
Henry Walter Bates, letter to
Darwin, 1861
Of all the
myriad varieties of physical adaptation that play a part in aiding survival,
two stand above the pale for their sophistication and a certain sly genius
concealed within their unmistakable utility: defense mechanisms and protective
(or cryptic) coloration.
Plant
and animal defenses are products of evolutionary
arms races. The first predator was
presumably a single celled organism that hit on the idea of engulfing some
smaller cell, thereby inventing “food.” Since that game-changing event
transpired, most living things have taken part in what amounts to an endless
contest in which winners manage to postpone being eaten—whether by means of
running faster, growing a thicker shell to deter stronger teeth, or simply
going into hiding. Means of defense go well beyond the no-frills stings and
bulky shells, the diverse spines and vile excretions. Some creatures rely on
the use of smoke screens, electrocution, and self-evisceration—forceful
displays of nature’s creative prowess. And among those slow-moving creatures
without such defenses, many styles of protective coloration exhibit a subtlety
of rendition that in some instances is almost beyond belief.
These
attributes have long been a source of wonder and cause for heated debate thanks
to a shared quality of overtly appearing designed,
in a manner that exceeds the “appearance” of design in other natural
features. Nonetheless, it is widely
held that these things can all be accounted for solely by the slow-turning
wheels of gradualistic evolution. In spite of this widespread conviction, to
this day there are no definitive explanations for marvels that have the appearance of being intentionally
shaped by an intelligent agency. And there are some good reasons to question
prevailing accounts.
Animals
marshal an array of stratagems to defend themselves in a world teeming with
sharp-eyed predators. These defenses also help relieve the stress of constantly
being on the lookout. In fact, many creatures can go about their lives
virtually unmolested thanks to their built-in defenses including what amount to
flashing warning signs.
Here is a partial listing of strategies,
including a few singular cases:
• Many varieties of poisonous
stings and bites. (Though used for securing prey these are often solely for protection.) Many insects are unpalatable
as a result of concentrating toxic compounds from food plants in their own flesh.
On the other hand, animals (such as frogs and newts) synthesize their own poisonous
chemicals.
• A fearsome assortment of
spines, spurs, spikes, and detachable quills. These can be poison-tipped or capable of causing potentially fatal
infections.
• Diverse noxious, sticky,
toxic (or just plain foul) excretions and emissions are employed throughout the animal kingdom. The chemical
elements are often altered versions of substances having other, completely unrelated physiological roles.
• Along with their remarkable
camouflage systems, cephalopods
(squid, octopuses, and cuttlefish) deploy opaque “smoke screens” by
expelling clouds of a black inky substance.
• Many lizards and salamanders can
voluntarily shed their tails, which (advantageously) then flail wildly, distracting the predator
while the real prize escapes…and grows a new tail.
• Sea cucumbers (a soft-bodied mollusk) perform a related trick. When
threatened they expel several internal organs. A portion of the
eviscerated tissue consists of tubular elements from the sea cucumber’s respiratory system. This
material expands many fold, elongating greatly and becoming sticky, disabling predators.
The organs are regenerated within days.
• Warning coloration: bright
colors or stripes, often in combination with black stripes or against a black background, which indicate toxicity
or other dangerous defenses.
• The electric eel (actually, a type of fish) can deliver brief shocks of
up to 850 volts.
• As well as being highly camouflaged, some species of the slow-moving horned lizards can spray blood for up to several feet from a special pore near the eyelid. In addition to the element of shock and surprise, their blood is reported to have a repellent effect.
• The bombardier beetle stores a mixture of hydroquinones and hydrogen peroxide in a special chamber near its anus. If threatened, the beetle empties the contents into another sealed chamber where the chemicals mix with a catalyst, triggering a violent reaction that raises the temperature of the mixture to near the boiling point. Resulting pressure forces the hot fluid through a turret-like nozzle that the beetle can aim in a chosenn each of its claws. The anemones’ stinging tentacles provide protection to the otherwise defenseless crab, which also scrapes off food particles captured in the tentacles for its own benefit.
• The bizarre Malaysian “exploding ant,”suicide bomber of the insect world. It belongs to a group of forest-dwelling ants that are often attacked by marauding weaver ants. Workers have hugely enlarged mandibular glands running the length of their bodies. As a last resort in battle, worker ants violently contract their abdominal muscles, causing the poison-filled mandibular glands to rupture, spraying a sticky and corrosive fluid in all directions. This toxic glue can ensnare all nearby attackers, immobilizing them.
• As well as being highly camouflaged, some species of the slow-moving horned lizards can spray blood for up to several feet from a special pore near the eyelid. In addition to the element of shock and surprise, their blood is reported to have a repellent effect.
• The bombardier beetle stores a mixture of hydroquinones and hydrogen peroxide in a special chamber near its anus. If threatened, the beetle empties the contents into another sealed chamber where the chemicals mix with a catalyst, triggering a violent reaction that raises the temperature of the mixture to near the boiling point. Resulting pressure forces the hot fluid through a turret-like nozzle that the beetle can aim in a chosenn each of its claws. The anemones’ stinging tentacles provide protection to the otherwise defenseless crab, which also scrapes off food particles captured in the tentacles for its own benefit.
• The bizarre Malaysian “exploding ant,”suicide bomber of the insect world. It belongs to a group of forest-dwelling ants that are often attacked by marauding weaver ants. Workers have hugely enlarged mandibular glands running the length of their bodies. As a last resort in battle, worker ants violently contract their abdominal muscles, causing the poison-filled mandibular glands to rupture, spraying a sticky and corrosive fluid in all directions. This toxic glue can ensnare all nearby attackers, immobilizing them.
Then there are the
“passive” protective devices of cryptic coloration. This encompasses both camouflage and mimicry, of which there are a number of distinct subcategories
(several being named after their discoverers). These include:
• Cryptism: The commonest form of camouflage: an organism resembles a
leaf, flower part, twig, rock, or the surface upon which
it typically resides or rests. With ever increasing frequency, photographic images are
appearing of exotic animals whose cryptism elicits gasps of amazement: walking-stick insects,
caterpillars, katydids, mantises…geckos, toads, bizarre sea-horses such as the leafy sea dragon and other reef-dwelling fishes.
• Active camouflage: Some animals can, through various physiological
means, alter their coloration to match different backgrounds.
• Disruptive coloration: Patterns of bold spots or stripes that serve
to “break up” an animal’s outline against a complex backdrop (as is
the case with leopards and zebras). In combination with cryptic coloration, an animal
“disappears” against its background.
• Batesian
mimicry: The “sheep in wolf’s clothing” trick: a harmless animal resembles
a toxic or dangerous one.
• Müllerian mimicry: Two or more unpalatable species come to resemble
one another, reinforcing their warning coloration patterns.
• Automimicry: One part of an organism’s body resembles another part
(snakes whose blunt-tipped tails resemble a head; butterflies
and other insects whose hind-ends mimic heads.) If attacked, they may escape with non-fatal
injuries. Also, various butterflies, moths, birds, and fish possess “eye-spots” which, when
presented, may startle a predator, allowing escape.
• Wassmannian mimicry: The mimic lives alongside the model (usually
social insects such as ants, bees, or termites) within its colony
or nest, producing pheromones that make their presence acceptable.
The phenomenon of
protective coloration has always elicited wonder and admiration and has
frequently been held up as proof of the existence of an intelligent creator.
Even after Darwin, certain scientists have admitted doubts as to whether a
naturalistic explanation could account for certain particularly vexing cases.
In fact, the discovery of mimicry (as a biological phenomenon) was considered
the first truly convincing demonstration of the powers of natural selection.
Henry Walter Bates, along
with Alfred Russel Wallace, embarked on an expedition to Brazil in 1848. Bates
and Wallace—only in their mid-twenties at the time—were both avid collectors of
beetles and butterflies, a fashionable hobby in Victorian England. Each had
been inspired by Darwin’s The Voyage of
the Beagle, first published in 1839. Insect collectors (Darwin being one himself) were ideally suited to
probe evolution’s mysteries, being particularly attuned to the variety and
extreme diversity of the creatures they studied. The question of how such
diversity arose was a much-discussed topic. Hoping to finally solve the problem
of the origin of species, the two young men traveled up the Amazon River,
together and then separately. Wallace, sick and exhausted, was forced to return
to England in 1852. On the journey home, Wallace’s ship caught fire and
sank—with all his specimens and notes. Bates remained abroad for seven more
years. Having collected almost 15,000 animal species (8000 of them unknown to
science) he arrived back in England, his body likewise ravaged by sickness and
deprivation. Just months later Origin of
Species was published. Bates
became an enthusiastic supporter of Darwin’s new theory and the two became fast
friends and life-long allies.
Bates’ most important
contribution was the discovery of a previously unrecorded form of mimicry that
now bears his name. In Brazil he
noted many instances of one butterfly
species that had taken on the colors and wing patterns of another. Typically,
these markings were patches and stripes of vivid primary colors against a dark
background (a key design theme in warning devices used by animals). After
careful study, Bates found that one of the two—he termed it the model—was avoided by predators due to a
diet of toxic plants while its mimic
was safe to eat. Even more intriguing: during his travels, Bates repeatedly
found distinct varieties of one of the unpalatable (model) species being
impersonated by different varieties of the same mimic species. To further
complicate matters, in some regions he found numbers of separate species (even
day-flying moths) all sporting similar flashy wing patterns. And all along the
way he came across instances of remarkable protective coloration. The sheer
complexity and degree of sophistication of all these things captivated him.
Back in England, Bates
promoted his findings as the clearest-yet demonstration of natural selection at
work. He described his discoveries in Darwinian terms, explaining them in words
similar to these: Through chance mutation,
some varieties of butterflies acquire a purely fortuitous, crude resemblance to
an unpalatable species. Over time this resemblance increasingly grows more
refined, with selection favoring the offspring of superior mimics. And ever
since, books have offered up the explanation proposed by Darwin and Bates as
consummate illustrations of natural selection in action. But is it as simple as
this? Time and again, such matters have proven to be more complex than
originally thought. Perhaps other influences are at work here.
Much has been learned about
butterfly wing patterns through laboratory rearing and cross-breeding
experiments. It was recognized that these colors and patterns on
two-dimensional surfaces (as opposed to more complex three-dimensional
structures) were more readily modified by genetic alteration. In the 1920s and
30s biologists determined that, in at least one major group, individual species’ markings were
variations on a basic ground plan
consisting of “pattern elements”—rows of stripes and spots confined to specific
wing-vein-bound subregions. An important observation was made: these subregions
are modular and, as such, can not
only evolve independently of other elements but do so readily. The established
theory explaining butterfly mimicry is based on a two step hypothesis.
Initially, mutations in those regulatory genes responsible for wing coloration
result in morphological changes that bestow a chance resemblance. The second
step involves further small scale mutations which, when subjected to natural
selection, progressively result in ever-closer resemblances. Modifications to
wing patterns, being modular and subject to point mutations (changes in a single nucleotide base), occur
readily. And often.
More recently—thanks to advances in evolutionary
developmental biology (evo-devo)—a fairly detailed understanding of the genetic
aspects of coloration and pattern development has been reached. Specific genes,
previously found in Drosophila and
known to have additional functions, were identified as being responsible for eye-spot
formation as well. Location of such features is set by the ground plan. When,
if, and for how long the genes are switched on determines whether any one locus
develops into a simple spot or something more elaborate—like one of those
markings that bears an astonishing resemblance to a vertebrate eye (complete
with a tiny silvery dot near its center to create the convincing illusion of a
reflection on an eye’s glassy surface).
While it is recognized that many such developmental
issues are far from settled, researchers expect to make great strides in coming
years. However, this conventional mechanistic approach to these matters will
not likely account for the evolutionary “need” for such things as the butterfly
wing mark that captures, with the realism of a trompe l’oeil painting, a beaded
raindrop—complete with a line on the wing that, where it passes beneath the
“raindrop,” is offset a bit to perfectly match the refraction such a spheroid
lens would produce.
Of all the graphic illustrations of the raw power
of organic evolution, I find specific instances of cryptic coloration to be among the
most amazing due to their refined subtleties, sheer ingenuity, and aesthetic
appeal: the squid-like cuttlefish, which can alter its color, pattern, and skin
texture in an instant to match virtually any backdrop…the wide variety of
insects that are essentially invisible against the leaves, bark, or ground upon
which they hide….those birds and mammals of northern latitudes that turn white
in the fall.[1] The
list is a long one. While so many people have wondered how such things have
come about, the answers offered by Darwinian theory paint a simple picture: The occasional minor, random mutation—a
frilly edged wing, a slightly different color, a seemingly inconsequential bump—provides
a chance advantage and, if passed on to offspring, improves (even if ever so
slightly) their odds of surviving. Through time, by way of additional random
changes, these features grow or are enhanced and become ever more subtly
realistic.
In the case of cryptism—bearing in mind the perils
of “the argument from personal incredulity”—I nonetheless find myself unmoved
by this stock explanation. In the case of the many forms of protective
coloration, however, it seems to lack real substance or a feel of scientific
authenticity. My use of such language clearly reveals a response that is intuitive and emotional and is thus
not a valid approach to a scientific question. Most people with scientific
backgrounds, aware of the issues, find the matter of cryptism not only unmystifying, but a model demonstration
of Darwinism in action.
One
particular experience when I was sixteen was pivotal in a burgeoning
comprehension that nature has certain qualities that defy rational explanation.
While hiking in a brushy canyon near my home, I came across a sizeable gopher
snake—at almost five feet long, the largest I had yet seen. (As to the
emotional impact of encounters with wild animals: when it comes to serpents,
“size matters.”)
The snake was hidden under an old
board and, with the sudden loss of cover, it speedily assumed a defensive
posture. I felt a rush of adrenaline when it coiled like a rattlesnake, head
reared and tongue flickering. Staring me down, the mostly harmless reptile had become a rattlesnake. The species has
brown markings with paler interiors on a golden-colored background so gopher
snakes look somewhat similar to their dangerous relatives to begin with.
However, the transformation of its appearance went beyond simply looking like a rattlesnake. Its head
became flattened and the jaws were flared—distinguishing viper traits. The
snake inflated its lungs, which had the effect of markedly increasing its
girth. (Rattlers are conspicuously thick-bodied snakes.) Its slender tail, held
aloft, vibrated in a hazy blur—nearly indistinguishable from the blur of
rapidly buzzing rattles. Simultaneously, air was forcefully expelled from the
snake’s lungs with a dull hiss that credibly reproduced a certain instantly
recognizable sound. The imposter faced me squarely, weaving its head slowly
from side to side. Those who have faced down an angry rattlesnake while on full
adenal alert will attest that the net effect triggers a compelling instinctive
fear; keeping one’s distance is an involuntary, unquestioned response. (Of
course, being an adolescent male of my species, it was compulsory that I locate
a stick and wave it in front of the snake’s face, provoking multiple vigorous
strikes.) All in all, the entire display was a thrilling demonstration of
plucky menace and I left completely cowed. The memory remains vivid.
Later, in view of what I had
witnessed, it was a shock to realize that in addition to the diamond shaped
pattern imitating that of pit vipers, this snake had displayed a total of seven
discrete traits and behaviors that copied ones displayed by its dangerous relative: the pile of sinuous coils…reared
head held flat, jaws flared…tail borne vertically behind the coils, tip raised
and vibrating in a blur…inflated body…exhaled air that sounded amazingly
similar to that viscerally fear-inducing rattle…the spirited strikes. Later I
learned that these things are all known defensive responses (and that many
gopher snakes have died by human hands as a result of their subterfuge).
However, the thing that most amazed me was the realization that all these
responses were unlearned and purely instinctive. Snakes do not rear their
young—no mother snake imparted these skills. Nor is it likely that gopher
snakes learn such things through observing their cousins. No: they are born
with this remarkable talent for mimicry. How
do they know? I pondered this question for years. And still do.
Few literary people are aware that the
acclaimed expatriate Russian novelist Vladamir Nabakov was equally famous among
butterfly fanciers around the globe. Prior to his career as a writer, Nabakov
was a curator of Lepidoptera (butterflies
and moths) at the Harvard Museum of
Comparative Zoology. A recognized expert in his field, Nabakov was mindful of
contemporary neo-Darwinian thought when he wrote this oft-quoted passage in his
autobiography Speak, Memory:
The mysteries of mimicry had a special
attraction for me. Its phenomena showed an artistic perfection usually
associated with man-wrought things. Consider the imitation of oozing poison by
bubblelike macules on a wing (complete with pseudo-refraction) or by glossy
yellow knobs on a chrysalis (“Don’t eat me—I have already been squashed,
sampled and rejected”)…. When a certain moth resembles a certain wasp in shape
and color, it also walks and moves its antennae in a waspish, unmothlike
manner. When a butterfly has to look like a leaf, not only are all the details
of a leaf beautifully rendered but markings mimicking grub-bored holes are
generously thrown in. “Natural selection,” in the Darwinian sense, could not
explain the miraculous coincidence of imitative aspect and imitative behavior,
nor could one appeal to the theory of “the struggle for life” when a protective
device was carried to a point of mimetic subtlety, exuberance, and luxury far
in excess of a predator’s power of appreciation.
Nabakov wrote about these
matters in a similar vein in several non-fiction works, often imparting the
same attitude of skepticism toward a Darwinian explanation. This has puzzled
many of those who knew Nabakov as a credible scientist, fully aware of the
heresy he was committing in suggesting there was something more than natural
selection at work. Nabakov was certainly not alone—being a member of that
skeptics camp he wondered how something that so clearly showed signs of
deliberate design could be the end result of slowly accumulating random
mutations. But through what naturalistic means does an animal manage to
contrive an invisibility cloak?
Despite claims to the contrary, this is a question that
has not yet been answered satisfactorily. But here is an excellent example of
how mimicry is explained, from Peter Forbes’ Dazzled and Deceived: Mimicry and Camouflage:
Very often
we fall into circular arguments when we speculate on evolution because there is
no purpose to it—no end in sight. We see that some organisms have survived, so
we say that these must have been the fittest. And which are ‘the fittest’?
Those which have survived. But in mimicry one creature has led and another has,
through selection, copied it. The old problem of attributing to nature a goal
when it has none dissolves in the face of mimicry because, although there is no
purpose to the whole of it, for the mimicking species there is a goal: to copy the model. So we have
an index of the success of evolution in producing the match. Similarly with the
butterflies that mimic dead leaves. Success is demonstrable.
This is a striking but not
atypical example of a phenomenon discussed in the last chapter: readers are
warned of circular reasoning’s pitfalls, which the unwary often fall prey to.
But the author proceeds to promptly stumble into that very trap. How can we explain camouflage and mimicry
from an evolutionary standpoint? Answer: Through the process of natural
selection working on genes. How do we
know that natural selection accounts for these features? Because, writes
Forbes, “in mimicry one creature has led and another has, through selection,
copied it.” Their success, as he asserts, “is demonstrable.”
Odd, that the fallacious reasoning so often glides by
unnoticed…that the feature or phenomenon in question is never actually
“explained.” Much progress has been made in describing mechanisms behind wing
pattern evolution and they are well-supported and make perfect sense. And yet,
the real issue—How are these mimicries achieved?—is never directly confronted. The
same is true of many remarkable instances of camouflage. How, through gradualistic processes, have genes been modified to the
extent that a butterfly so perfectly resembles a fallen leaf that it even
includes gnawed-looking edges and dark patches that appear exactly like the decayed
places on a dead leaf? What “codes” for a tiny, perfectly placed silver dot
that imparts a sense of the moist reflectivity of an eye’s surface? And
although this is a different matter, I wonder how moths and other insects that
rely on a specific body-axis orientation when hiding on a background of leaf or
bark have learned to rotate their bodies so that instead of conspicuously
standing out they vanish into thick tropical air. In some instances, this
behavior also involves a specific way of positioning legs and antennae. These are simply inherited, instinctive
behaviors, we are informed. No doubt this is correct but the fact remains
that inherited behaviors—one of biology’s great mysteries—have continually
eluded scientific explanation.
Other unanswered queries: Why, if protective coloration is so readily achieved by way of modular
genetic mutation and selection, do more species not “take advantage”of such a
highly effective adaptive capacity? And this: If Batesian mimicry is to remain a successful tactic, the mimic has to be significantly less abundant than the model. (If
mimics were as common or more abundant than their models, predators “sampling”
both species would not learn which were to be avoided.) So how are populations of Batesian mimics world-wide regulated to
maintain rarity relative to the populations of their models? Another
question, one of those posed by Nabakov: It is widely recognized that specific
instances of camouflage and mimicry are often not perfect and need not be so to
succeed. Why then does natural selection,
throw in such unbelievably subtle and apparently superfluous details? Not
only has my research failed to yield answers to these hard questions but thus
far I have yet to find askers.
Such puzzles are little
more than distraction in the face of a deeper issue. The phenomena of defense
mechanisms and protective coloration are directly entwined with the survival
imperative. They recapitulate in the clearest fashion to what lengths life will go in order to further
survival. Life always finds a way and
its solutions are always something to behold. In the cases of camouflage and
mimicry, the artistry and sophistication and sheer variety of the material
devices are one of the stronger demonstrations of Natural Design’s creative imperative.
Almost all forms of protective coloration show signs of nature’s signature
flair, its tendency toward over-embellishment and whimsy combined—paradoxically—with
spareness and practicality. And, always, there is that thing Nabakov (with his
uncanny knack for language) called “exuberance.” Natural selection, in contrast, is a conceptual tool that makes no attempt
to the underscore the enigmatic richness and depth that are immaterial elements
of all biological features.
By the same token, positing
Natural Design as an explanation goes no further toward elucidating the
mechanisms involved. It makes no such claims, being little more than a
guidepost. Natural Design offers a way of viewing nature in its entirety as
having a capacity to elicit broad-brush changes and somehow speed the arrival
of crucial structures and vital processes. Its subtle influence has helped
bring about life’s greatest
necessities—things like ribosomes and chloroplasts, respiration and
photosynthesis, retractable claws and binocular vision.
As with many
of nature’s other countenances, in its highly successful expressions of
camouflage and mimicry life becomes (or at least mimics) art. In the case of
such utilitarian features, this seems an over-the-top extravagance, a needless
addition of fringe and filigree. Perhaps what some of us find a bit unnerving
about this esthetic profligacy is the lingering suspicion that this particular
form of natural beauty bears a veiled message. What we attempt to explain in
utilitarian terms exists in nature as something more than simple practicality.
Meanwhile, we are staring directly at the biological message while it remains
invisible…hiding in plain sight.
©2017 by Tim Forsell draft 15 Oct 2017
[1] This crucial adaptation has been independently
“discovered” by ptarmigan, foxes and wolves, hares, and weasels (the mammals
being in three separate orders). The mechanisms behind the process have not yet
been identified or explained aside from there being a connection to day length.
No comments:
Post a Comment